Serveur © IRCAM - CENTRE POMPIDOU 1996-2005. Tous droits réservés pour tous pays. All rights reserved. |
ICMC: International Computer Music Conference, Thessaloniki, Greece, Septembre 1997
Copyright © Ircam - Centre Georges-Pompidou 1997
Roughness was originally defined by Helmholtz [6] to provide a sensory basis for musical consonance. A basic feature of Western tonal harmony is to distinguish within the ensemble of possible intervals obtainable with the chromatic scale those that are considered consonant from those that are considered dissonant. This distinction is of course by no mean rigid, as shown by the progressive shift from dissonance to consonance of some intervals throughout the history of music. However, as has been noted since Pythagoras, intervals made of simple integer ratios are situated at one extreme of the scale (consonant) whereas the more complex ratios are considered dissonant. Numerous theories have been proposed to account for this enigma, and Helmholtz provides one of them: as the ratio between two sounds involves more complex integers, more partials will be ``mistuned'' and create roughness. Since the dissonance of Western tonal intervals corresponds to their roughness, this attribute may be proposed as a sensory basis upon which the complex rules of harmony were built. Cross-cultural studies involving American and Japanese listeners [3] did show some agreement on judgments of simple isolated intervals according to their consonance.
Along with other factors such as melodic motion or implicit knowledge of tonal pitch space hierarchy, dissonance is considered to play a part in the expression of an important feature of tonal music: tension and release movements. A dissonant chord is considered unstable, calling for a resolution on a more consonant chord. As a potential source of sensory dissonance, roughness was shown [2] to play a part in the expression of these schemas in tonal contexts.
A correspondence between a sensory feature and a musical notion takes on a new interest when listeners are confronted to music not leaning on internalized syntactic rules. In such a case, a previous study [10] showed that tension movements could nevertheless clearly be perceived by musician or non-musician listeners. Roughness appeared as strongly correlated to the tension judgments. As the material tested had no harmonic tonal function, its roughness value could provide a relevant indication for the composer. Naturally, music is (generally) more than a juxtaposition of sound events and a relative roughness value of an isolated chord will not determine a harmonic function in the time course of a piece. Nevertheless, roughness estimation of musical material can be, in our view, a relevant parameter in the framework of computer-aided composition environments.
A different approach consisted in studying the influence of temporal parameters, by means of amplitude-modulated stimuli. Terhardt [11] demonstrated a dependence of roughness on the frequency and depth of the modulation. He also found that differences in roughness can be measured for sounds having the same amplitude spectrum but different waveform envelopes. The hypothesis he advanced is that roughness is determined by the envelope fluctuations of the signal within an auditory filter. Models have therefore been developed in which roughness estimates are based on the magnitude of temporal fluctuations of the signal envelope after auditory filtering ([4] for instance).
In the general case, the spectral and temporal cues are strongly covariant. An experiment has therefore been designed to oppose them in order to provide quantitative experimental data relevant to a discussion of the concurrent approaches. In addition, some authors claimed that roughness differences could be heard for sounds having the same envelope and frequency composition. This point, still lacking experimental evidence, will be addressed as well.
Seven series of pAM stimuli were created in which a given center frequency and ``modulation'' frequency were associated. In each case, m was chosen to produce maximum roughness for a pure tone of frequency c modulated sinusoidally at m. The phase of the central component was varied from - /2 to + /2 in steps of /6. For each series, seven stimuli were thus produced with four different envelopes corresponding to the four absolute phase differences (characterized hereafter by their rms value).
Stimuli were presented over headphones to listeners. Center frequencies were arranged in randomized separate blocks, and within a block all 42 pairs of nonidentical stimuli in both orders were tested. For each pair, listeners were asked to decide which sound was rougher (2AFC). The notion of roughness was introduced with the help of amplitude-modulated sounds the modulation depth of which the listener could vary in a continuous fashion, thus changing concomitantly the amplitude spectrum and envelope. Two groups of 15 listeners were presented two different sets of cs.
The Bradley-Terry-Luce (BTL) method was used to construct a roughness scale derived from the binary paired-comparison judgments. In order to test whether differences between scale values are significant, they must be compared to the standard deviation of the results. The standard deviations were estimated nonparametrically by the bootstrap technique. The results for the seven series of stimuli are presented in Figure 1.
Figure 1. Results for the seven stimulus series corresponding to different center frequencies. The roughness value estimated by the BTL method is plotted as a function of the rms value of the amplitude envelope of the signal. Solid lines represent > 0 and dashed lines represent < 0.
The relatively small size of the standard deviations indicates a strong agreement across subjects. The most obvious factor contributing to roughness is the envelope rms, greater envelope rms corresponding to higher roughness values. This effect is similar in range across most cs tested although there is a decrease in range for low cs. However, for cs at or below 4 kHz there is also a strong effect of the sign of , indicating a significant contribution of phase for stimuli with the same overall envelope and the same amplitude spectrum. This effect disappears for all cs at = /2 and for all phases for c = 8 kHz.
The observed significant influence of phase for a given envelope and amplitude spectrum is not expected with the envelope fluctuation hypothesis. A spectral interpretation may be proposed to account for this result: combination tones (CTs) generated by the three components of the pAM stimuli may have played a role in the roughness judgments. Among these tones, the first order cubic difference situated at 2(c)-(c+m) = c -m interacts with the acoustic component at the same frequency. Its phase and amplitude change as a function of phi, which may explain the differences observed in perceived roughness [1]. This interpretation nevertheless raises two questions. First, the phase effect disappears at c= 8 kHz, whereas combination tones are likely to be still present. To accommodate this finding, one would have to accept that the phase of the CTs does not change with the phase of the primaries in this high frequency region. Second, a large intersubject variability is generally observed in the phase dependence of the combination tone on the phases of the primaries. Our results display on the contrary a remarkable agreement between listeners.
A second framework consists in trying to link the roughness judgments to the temporal fine structure differences between stimuli. To assess these differences, a computational model that mimics the auditory system was used. A large variety of these models exist that present very similar characteristics. We chose to generate ``neural activity patterns'' by means of compression, rectification and 2-dimensional adaptive thresholding of the output of a gammatone filter-bank [8]. These patterns are intended to simulate the neural responses (along the cochlea) to the stimuli. Examination of the simulations indeed showed that the patterns produced by the opposite phases had different shapes. In the positive phase condition, the modulation shows a well defined maximum, that resembles the zero-lag condition, whereas the negative condition displays a ``smoother'' shape. This difference disappears for the /2 condition, as well as for the highest carrier frequency where temporal fine structure following by the auditory system is impossible. A periodicity coding mechanism based on detection of maxima would be affected by such a change. Recent results obtained in the field of comparative psychology [5] indicated that members of other species also seem to be sensitive to auditory roughness. Therefore, it may be plausible that roughness derives from ubiquitous physiological mechanisms such as periodicity coding. An alternative interpretation of the results could therefore be that the temporal fine structure of the modulation present at the level of primary auditory nerve fibers has an influence on roughness perception.
[2]
E. Bigand, R. Parncutt, and F. Lerdahl.
Perception of musical tension in short chord sequences : the
influence of harmonic function, sensory dissonance, horizontal motion, and
musical training.
Perception and Psychophysics, 58(1):125--141, 1996.
[3]
P. G. Butler, J. W. amd Daston.
Musical consonance as musical preference: a cross-cultural study.
Journal of General Psychology, 79:129--142, 1963.
[4]
P. Daniel and R. Weber.
Psychoacoustical roughness: Implementation of an optimized model.
Acustica, 83:113--123, 1997.
[5] S. H. Hulse, D. J. Bernard, and
R. F. Braaten. Auditory discrimination of chord-based
spectral structures by european starlings (sturnus vulgaris).
J. Exp. Psych.: Gen, 124(4):409--423, 1995.
[6]
Hermann L. F. von Helmholtz.
On the Sensations of Tone as the Physiological Basis for the
Theory of Music.
1885. 2nd ed. trans. A. J. Ellis, from German 4th ed. (1885), Reprinted,
Dover, New York (1954).
[7]
W. Hutchinson and L. Knopoff.
The acoustic component of Western consonance.
Interface, 7:1--29, 1978.
[8]
R. D. Patterson, M. H. Allerhand, and C. Giguere.
Time-domain modelling of peripheral auditory processing: A modular
architecture and a software platform.
J. Acoust. Soc. Am., 98:1890--1894, 1995.
[9] R. Plomp and W. Levelt. Tonal
consonance and critical bandwidth. {\em
J. Acoust. Soc. Am.}, 38:548--560, 1965.
[10]
D. Pressnitzer, S. McAdams, S. Winsberg, and J. Fineberg.
Roughness and musical tension of orchestral timbres.
In Proceedings of the 4th International Conference on Music
Perception and Cognition, pages 85--90, Montréal, Canada, August 1996.
[11]
E. Terhardt.
On the perception of periodic sound fluctuation (roughness).
Acustica, 30:201--212, 1974.
____________________________ ____________________________
Server © IRCAM-CGP, 1996-2008 - file updated on .
Serveur © IRCAM-CGP, 1996-2008 - document mis à jour le .